In biology, a weapon is a specialized physical trait that is used by animals to compete with other individuals for resources.[1] Most commonly, the term refers to structures that males use to fight other males off for access to mates.[2] They can also be used to defend resources in intraspecific competition, or to ward off predators. Examples of weapons include horns and antlers, both among the most recognizable weapons, though even within those categories, the structure of the specific weaponry is often unique to the species, with a wide variety of designs observed across many genera.[3]

The impala uses horns to compete for resources.

Many weapons evolve through sexual selection, as they are most often used to fight off competitors for access to mates.[3] A mate is won in battle either by a male chasing off a fellow competitor or killing it, usually leaving the victor as the only option for the female to reproduce with, favoring males with particularly effective weaponry.[4] More broadly, weaponry in animals may consist of any specialized morphology that is present within an organism to aid in its advantage against rivals.[1] Many hypotheses have been produced by researchers to possibly explain the mechanisms behind the evolution of weapons, with studies detailing the intensity, duration, and conclusion of intraspecific combat, as well as analyzing the rapid diversification within species.[5] 

Since Darwin's publication The Descent of Man, extensive research has been done on the presence of agonistic behavior and the usage of animal weaponry by different species.[1] Weaponry displays in animals have been found to increase their likelihood of survival in different ways, such as when interacting with other individuals or trying to find another mate, or to defend against predators.[2]        

Occurrence

edit
 
The horn-like projections of a chameleon are used to fight other males on tree trunks.

Weapons are common among many genera of animals. Among vertebrates, they are most often found in mammals and fish, and are also known to occur in reptiles, though far less commonly in that class. Many species of dinosaur, an extinct clade of vertebrates, also possessed weapons. Arthropods, such as arachnids and crabs, also have species that wield weapons, and they are extraordinarily widespread among insects. [5]

In mammals, weapons are common and take a number of diverse forms. They are most common among the ungulates.[5] Antlers, complex and unique weapons that are an extension of an animal's skull, are found only among male deer, ungulates who are even-toed ruminants.[6] Rhinoceroses are even-toed ungulates which have horns made of keratin; both sexes use these horns in contests with other individuals of the same species.[7] Walruses and elephants, non-ungulates, both possess elongated tusks. Apart from mammals, the only other group of land vertebrates that shows widespread adoption of weapons are the chameleons, who possess horn-like structures for fighting over access to mates.[5]

 
The horns of a rhinoceros beetle are an example of one of the many weapons insects possess.

Some species of fish have weapons, though these traits are not as widespread as in mammals. Sawfish are named for their long rostrum, which can be used to inflict damage on other fish.[8] Unicornfish may also use their strange horn-like forehead protection as a weapon, although the general use of this and many similar structures in fish are still somewhat enigmatic. [5] Male salmon notably feature intraspecific competition for mates, and they use their elongated and toothy jaw to fight other males, both over access to females and over access to breeding sites.[5][9][10]

Weapons are very widespread among insects, having been observed in nearly every major taxonomic group.[5] Insects possess a huge variety of weapons, often with entirely different uses and modes of action. Rhinoceros beetles, like the mammal they are named for, have large horns which they famously use to fight for mates.[11][12] Harlequin beetles have legs that are longer than their entire body, using which they protect a suitable spot for a female to lay eggs. They also possess strong mandibles to bite other males, which can include removing their legs.[13] Camel crickets use spines on their tibias for two reasons; (1) to fight other males for access to females, and (2) to pin mates as a form of coercion.[2][14] Among arthropods that are not insects, male fiddler crabs have large claws that comprise half their body mass and are used to attract mates elaborate waving displays with the claw.[15] They are also, though somewhat less commonly, used as a weapon to directly attack other males.[16][5]

Evolution

edit
 
The horns that give Triceratops its name were used as weapons.

Animal weaponry is capable of drastic and rapid diversification in form, with closely related species, even within the same clade, having distinctly different weaponry.[5] One hypothesized mechanism for this is the gradual change in weaponry function from purely physical agonistic behavior, to a stronger emphasis on display, resulting in potentially more elaborate weapons. For instance, many dinosaurs may have had structures that were once weapons but were later used as ornaments to attract mates.[5] Another possible mechanism is the presence of male-male combat, with diverging lineages of animals expanding into different habitats and then fighting under different conditions. Changes in the physical contest between species in different environments may potentially drive the evolution in modified weaponry. Sites that have localized, defendable resources, like a single food or water source, or a breeding burrow, often are where species with weapons are found.[5]

Sexual selection has been a main focus on weaponry and antagonist interactions between animals, with males that present the largest weapons having the highest probability of winning.[17] Sexual selection has been credited by previous researchers as the main influence of nature's extravagant weaponry for the purpose of attractive females.[5] During agonistic interactions, there is the potential risk of weaponry lost resulting in a possible decrease in an individual's fighting ability and overall fitness.[17]

 
A trilobite of the genus Ceratarges, showing large spines probably used as weapons.

Most animal weapons probably emerged independently. The weapons of ungulates, for instance, are believed to have evolved independently during the Age of Mammals, and are not from an earlier, basal clade of mammals. One group that had weapons early in the history of multicellular life were the trilobites, one of the earliest arthropods, and a dominant life form in the Paleozoic.[18][5] Many species had horns or spikes which are theorized to have been used in intraspecific combat.[5] Many dinosaurs also had weapons such as spines, spikes, and plates, although the exact use of these is not known for all species—some may have been used for mating displays more than as weapons.[5]

An exception to the general independence of weapon evolution is found in stag beetles, whose common ancestor is believed to have had mandibles, much like modern stag beetles. However, that species likely had little or no sexual dimorphism in the mandible phenotype, unlike modern stag beetles, in which males usually have markedly larger mandibles than females.[19][5] Dung beetles, on the other hand, have evolved and lost their weapons many, many times over their history,[5] with the single genus Onthophagus, whose ancestor likely had horns, undergoing at least ten evolutionary events where horns were gained or lost.[20]

Usage & Characteristics

edit

Agonistic behaviors amongst animals for resources have been studied by many researchers, and specifically the interaction of weapons during these exchanges of behavior.[1] Not only the presence of weaponry, but also specific characteristics of the weaponry itself can have an effect on the outcome of competition in determining the winners and losers from an intraspecies competition.[5] The display of weaponry has been found to be favored in animals that frequently engage in contest as a mechanism to decrease the costs of aggression.[5]

 
A fiddler crab uses its large claw as both a display ornament for females, and a weapon to directly compete with other males.

Most weapons that are studied are utilize to injure other individuals. These include the most famous weapons, such as antlers, horns, and mandibles; conversely, some animals have specialized "weapons" that actually fulfill a defensive role, such as the horned weevil, which have sheaths that can neutralize the horns of other beetles.[2][21] Some of these weapons can be used to inject chemicals into enemies; tentacles and stingers are examples of this phenomenon.[2] Other weapons are used to displace their opponents,[1] usually to block burrows or entrances from other individuals. Some termites, for instance, use their heads as plugs to physically impede other termites from invading through the colony entrance.[2] These termites also use their mandibles to displace enemies.[22] Finally, many weapons are used for displays, with males using their large physical features to attract females to mate with.[2][5] Additionally, most organisms display their weapons before fighting as a threat to other individuals.[1] In general, displays are considered any behavior that is used to show a species' fighting ability without any physical contact, and the term applies to signalling both potential mates and potential opponents. This behavior allows for rivals to have the chance to assess the weaponry that is present to determine whether to engage in physical agonistic behavior or not.[1]

Female Weaponry

edit

Since the publication of The Descent of Man and The Origin of Species, research has been heavily focused on weaponry in male animals, potentially leaving out females and their possible mechanisms behind present weaponry.[23] Presence of weapons in females, and female-female competition has been seen in many species for better fitness of both sexual and social selection.[24][23] Social selection, first hypothesized by Mary Jane West-Eberhard, is a broader term that includes both sexual and non sexual behaviors to increase an individual's fitness.[23] For example, research has suggested that horns present in female bovids, may have evolved from competition for resources such as food.[24] 

edit

 

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c d e f g Palaoro, Alexandre V.; Peixoto, Paulo Enrique Cardoso (2022). "The hidden links between animal weapons, fighting style, and their effect on contest success: a meta-analysis". Biological Reviews. 97 (5): 1948–1966. doi:10.1111/brv.12877. ISSN 1464-7931. PMID 35790073. S2CID 250282536.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g Lane, Sarah M (2018-07-19). "What is a weapon?". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 58 (6): 1055–1063. doi:10.1093/icb/icy083. hdl:10026.1/16293. ISSN 1540-7063. PMID 30010939.
  3. ^ a b Emberts, Zachary; Hwang, Wei Song; Wiens, John J. (2021-01-27). "Weapon performance drives weapon evolution". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 288 (1943): 2020–2898. doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.2898. PMC 7893261. PMID 33499793.
  4. ^ Krebs, J. R., and Nicholas B. Davies. "Sexual Selection, Sperm Competition and Sexual Conflict." An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology. 4th ed. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, 2012.
  5. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t Emlen, Douglas J. (2008-12-01). "The Evolution of Animal Weapons". Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 39 (1): 387–413. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173502. ISSN 1543-592X.
  6. ^ Lincoln, G. A. (March 1992). "Biology of antlers". Journal of Zoology. 226 (3): 517–528. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1992.tb07495.x.
  7. ^ Berger, Joel; Cunningham, Carol (June 1998). "Natural variation in horn size and social dominance and their importance to the conservation of black rhinoceros". Conservation Biology. 12 (3): 708–711. doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.1998.97207.x. JSTOR 2387253.
  8. ^ Wueringer, Barbara E.; Squire, Lyle; Collin, S. P. (2009-03-19). "The biology of extinct and extant sawfish (Batoidea: Sclerorhynchidae and Pristidae)". Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries. 19 (4): 445–464. Bibcode:2009RFBF...19..445W. doi:10.1007/s11160-009-9112-7.
  9. ^ Fleming, Ian A. (Sep 1994). "Captive breeding and the conservation of wild salmon populations". Conservation Biology. 8 (3): 886–888. Bibcode:1994ConBi...8..886F. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1994.08030863-13.x. JSTOR 2386541.
  10. ^ Quinn, Thomas P. (Mar 1999). "Variation in Pacific salmon reproductive behaviour associated with species, sex and levels of competition". Behaviour. 136 (2): 179–204. doi:10.1163/156853999501270.
  11. ^ Karino, Kenji; Niiyama, Hisatsugu; Chiba, Mutsumi (2005). "Horn length is the determining factor in the outcomes of escalated fights among male Japanese horned beetles, Allomyrina dichotoma L.(Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae)". Journal of Insect Behavior. 18 (6): 805–815. Bibcode:2005JIBeh..18..805K. doi:10.1007/s10905-005-8741-5.
  12. ^ Buchalski, Benjamin; Gutierrez, Eric; Emlen, Douglas; Lavine, Laura; Swanson, Brook (15 October 2019). "Variation in an Extreme Weapon: Horn Performance Differences across Rhinoceros Beetle (Trypoxylus dichotomus) Populations". Insects. 10 (10): 346. doi:10.3390/insects10100346. PMC 6835817. PMID 31618906.
  13. ^ Zeh, David W.; Zeh, Jeanne A.; Tavakilian, Gerard (1992). "Sexual Selection and Sexual Dimorphism in the Harlequin Beetle Acrocinus longimanus". Biotropica. 24 (1): 86–96. Bibcode:1992Biotr..24...86Z. doi:10.2307/2388476. JSTOR 2388476.
  14. ^ Haley, Estenia L.; Gray, David A. (2011-11-09). "Mating Behavior and Dual-Purpose Armaments in a Camel Cricket". Ethology. 118 (1): 49–56. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01985.x.
  15. ^ Perez, D. M., Rosenberg, M. S., and Pie, M. R., 2012, The evolution of waving displays in fiddler crabs (Uca spp., Crustacea: Ocypodidae): Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, v. 106, p. 307-315.
  16. ^ Callander, S., Kahn, A. T., Maricic, T., Jennions, M. D., and Backwell, P. R. Y., 2013, Weapons or mating signals? Claw shape and mate choice in a fiddler crab: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, v. 67, p. 1163-1167.
  17. ^ a b Emberts, Zachary; St. Mary, Colette M.; Herrington, Tyler J.; Miller, Christine W. (2018-05-03). "Males missing their sexually selected weapon have decreased fighting ability and mating success in a competitive environment". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 72 (5): 81. doi:10.1007/s00265-018-2494-6. ISSN 1432-0762. S2CID 253820366.
  18. ^ Aria, Cédric (2022-04-26). "The origin and early evolution of arthropods". Biological Reviews. 97 (5): 1786–1809. doi:10.1111/brv.12864.
  19. ^ Hosoya, Tadatsugu; Araya, Kunio (2005). "Phylogeny of Japanese stag beetles (Coleoptera: Lucanidae) inferred from 16S mtrRNA gene sequences, with reference to the evolution of sexual dimorphism of mandibles". Zoological Science. 22 (12): 1305–1318. doi:10.2108/zsj.22.1305. PMID 16462103.
  20. ^ Emlen, Douglas J.; Marangelo, Jennifer; Ball, Bernard; Cunningham, Clifford W. (2005). "Diversity in the Weapons of Sexual Selection: Horn Evolution in the beetle genus Onthophagus (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae)". Evolution. 59 (5): 1060–1084. doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb01044.x. PMID 16136805.
  21. ^ Eberhard, William G.; Garcia-C., J. Mauricio; Lobo, Jorge (2000). "Size–specific defensive structures in a horned weevil confirm a classic battle plan: avoid fights with larger opponents". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 267 (1448): 1129–1134. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1118. PMC 1690650. PMID 10885518.
  22. ^ Matsuura, Kenji (2002). "Colony-level stabilization of soldier head width for head-plug defense in the termite Reticulitermes speratus (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 51 (2): 172–179. doi:10.1007/s00265-001-0426-2.
  23. ^ a b c Tobias, Joseph A.; Montgomerie, Robert; Lyon, Bruce E. (2012-08-19). "The evolution of female ornaments and weaponry: social selection, sexual selection and ecological competition". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367 (1600): 2274–2293. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0280. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 3391421. PMID 22777016.
  24. ^ a b Stankowich, Theodore; Caro, Tim (2009). "Evolution of Weaponry in Female Bovids". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 276 (1677): 4329–4334. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1256. ISSN 0962-8452. JSTOR 40506066. PMC 2817105. PMID 19759035.