Elephant communication

Elephants communicate via touching, visual displays, vocalisations, seismic vibrations, and semiochemicals.

Asian elephants greeting each other by inter-twining their trunks

Tactile

edit
 
Elephant (Loxodonta africana) mating ritual composite, Addo Elephant Park, South Africa

Individual elephants greet each other by stroking or wrapping their trunks; the latter also occurs during mild competition. Older elephants use trunk-slaps, kicks, and shoves to discipline younger ones. Individuals of any age and sex will touch each other's mouths, temporal glands, and genitals, particularly during meetings or when excited. This allows individuals to pick up chemical cues. Touching is especially important for mother–calf communication. When moving, elephant mothers will touch their calves with their trunks or feet when side-by-side or with their tails if the calf is behind them. If a calf wants to rest, it will press against its mother's front legs and when it wants to suckle, it will touch her breast or leg.[1]

Visual

edit

Visual displays mostly occur in agonistically behavioural situations. Elephants will try to appear more threatening by raising their heads and spreading their ears. They may add to the display by shaking their heads and snapping their ears, as well as throwing dust and vegetation. They are usually bluffing when performing these actions. Excited elephants may raise their trunks. Submissive ones will lower their heads and trunks, as well as flatten their ears against their necks, while those that accept a challenge will position their ears in a V shape.[1]

Acoustic

edit

Elephants produce several sounds, usually through the larynx, though some may be modified by the trunk.[1][2][3][4][5] Perhaps the most well-known call is the trumpet which is made by blowing through the trunk. Trumpeting is made during excitement, distress or aggression.[6][7][8][9] Fighting elephants may roar or squeal, and wounded ones may bellow.[10][11]

Asian elephants have been recorded to make three basic sounds: growls, squeaks, and snorts. Growls in their basic form are used for short-distance communication. During mild arousal, growls resonate in the trunk and become rumbles while for long-distance communication, they escalate into roars. Low-frequency growls are infrasonic and made in many contexts.[4] Squeaks come in two forms; chirpings and trumpets. Chirping consists of multiple short squeaks and signal conflict and nervousness. Trumpets are longer squeaks with increased loudness and produced during extreme arousal. Snorts signal changes in activity and increase in loudness during mild or strong arousal. During the latter case, when an elephant bounces the tip of the trunk it creates booms which serve as threat displays.[12]: 142 

Using machine learning, it was postulated in 2023, that elephants use personal names.[13]

Infrasound

edit

Elephants can produce infrasonic calls which occur at frequencies less than 20 Hz.[14] Infrasonic calls are important, particularly for long-distance communication,[1] in both Asian and African elephants. For Asian elephants, these calls have a frequency of 14–24 Hz, with sound pressure levels of 85–90 dB and last 10–15 seconds.[15] For African elephants, calls range from 15 to 35 Hz with sound pressure levels as high as 117 dB, allowing communication for many kilometres, with a possible maximum range of around 10 km (6 mi).[16]

Low frequency rumble visualised with acoustic camera.

At Amboseli National Park several different infrasonic calls have been identified:[12]: 145 

  • Greeting rumble – is emitted by adult females members of a family group that have united after having been separated for several hours.
  • Contact call – soft, unmodulated sounds made by an individual that has been separated from the groups have 2 km (1.2 mi).
  • Contact answer – made in response to the contact call; starts out loud, but softens toward the end.
  • "Let's go" rumble – a soft rumble emitted by the matriarch to signal to the other herd members that it is time to move to another spot.
  • Musth rumble – distinctive, low-frequency pulsated rumble emitted by musth males (nicknamed the "motorcycle").
  • Female chorus – a low-frequency, modulated chorus produced by several cows in response to a musth rumble.
  • Postcopulatory call – made by an oestrous cow after mating.
  • Mating pandemonium – calls of excitement made by a cow's family after she has mated.

Names and recognition

edit

In 2024, research published in Nature Ecology and Evolution demonstrated that elephants call each other by name and respond when they hear others call their name. Researchers analyzed hundreds of elephant calls recorded over more than a year in Kenya, utilizing machine learning to identify specific sounds made by elephants when calling each other. When recorded calls were played back, elephants responded to the sound of their friends or family members calling their name by either calling back or moving toward the speaker. This behavior suggests that elephants may be capable of abstract thought and possess a vocabulary that extends beyond names. It is suggested that this research could potentially enable humans to communicate directly with elephants in the future, possibly warning them about dangers such as poachers.[17]

Anatomy of the vocal tract

edit

The larynx of the elephant is the largest known among mammals. The vocal folds are long and are attached close to the epiglottis base. When comparing an elephant's vocal folds to those of a human, an elephant's are longer, thicker, and have a larger cross-sectional area. In addition, they are tilted at 45 degrees and positioned more anteriorly than a human's vocal folds.[18] From various experiments, the elephant larynx is shown to produce various and complex vibratory phenomena. During in vivo situations, these phenomena could be triggered when the vocal folds and vocal tract interact to raise or lower the fundamental frequency.[14]

One of the vibratory phenomena that occurred inside the larynx is alternating A-P (anterior-posterior) and P-A traveling waves, which happened due to the unusual larynx layout. This can be characterized by its unique glottal opening/closing pattern. When the trachea is at a pressure of approximately 6 kPa, phonation begins in the larynx and the laryngeal tissue starts to vibrate at approximately 15 kPa. Vocal production mechanisms at certain frequencies are similar to that of humans and other mammals and the laryngeal tissues are subjected to self-maintained oscillations. Two biomechanical features can trigger these traveling wave patterns, which are a low fundamental frequency and in the vocal folds, increasing longitudinal tension.[18]

Seismics

edit

Elephants are known to communicate with seismics, vibrations produced by impacts on the earth's surface or acoustical waves that travel through it. They appear to rely on their leg and shoulder bones to transmit the signals to the middle ear. When detecting seismic signals, the animals lean forward and put more weight on their larger front feet; this is known as the "freezing behaviour". Elephants possess several adaptations suited for seismic communication. The cushion pads of the feet contain cartilaginous nodes and have similarities to the acoustic fat found in marine mammals such as toothed whales and sirenians. A unique sphincter-like muscle around the ear canal constricts the passageway, thereby dampening acoustic signals and allowing the animal to hear more seismic signals.[19]

Elephants appear to use seismics for a number of purposes. An individual running or mock charging can create seismic signals that can be heard at great distances.[20] When detecting the seismics of an alarm call signalling danger from predators, elephants enter a defensive posture and family groups will pack together. Seismic waveforms produced by locomotion appear to travel distances of up to 32 km (20 mi) while those from vocalisations travel 16 km (10 mi).[21]

Semiochemicals

edit

Elephants can also communicate through olfaction and semiochemicals.[22][23] Secretion of semiochemicals can occur through feces and urine[24] as well as the temporal gland, a structure that is derived from sweat glands and located on both sides of the head of male and female elephants.[22][23] The substance secreted by male elephants from their temporal glands during musth contains many chemicals and seems to be of interest to females.[22] Elephants may investigate and detect semiochemicals through the vomeronasal organ (VNO).[23] Elephants may go through several steps of investigating the smell of a surface with their trunk before inserting its tip into their mouth to touch the anterior part of their hard palate and thus transfer semiochemicals to the VNO.[23]

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c d Payne, K. B.; Langauer, W. B (2000). "Elephant Communication". In Shoshani, J. (ed.). Elephants: Majestic Creatures of the Wild. Rodale Press. pp. 116–23. ISBN 978-0-87596-143-9. OCLC 475147472.
  2. ^ Stoeger, Angela S.; Baotic, Anton; Heilmann, Gunnar (August 2021). "Vocal Creativity in Elephant Sound Production". Biology. 10 (8): 750. doi:10.3390/biology10080750. ISSN 2079-7737. PMC 8389636. PMID 34439982.
  3. ^ Beeck, Veronika C.; Heilmann, Gunnar; Kerscher, Michael; Stoeger, Angela S. (2021-06-17). "A novel theory of Asian elephant high-frequency squeak production". BMC Biology. 19 (1): 121. doi:10.1186/s12915-021-01026-z. ISSN 1741-7007. PMC 8210382. PMID 34134675.
  4. ^ a b "Mystery of elephant infrasounds revealed". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  5. ^ Joyce, Hoope (January 2011). "The behavioral context of African elephant acoustic communication". In C. J. Moss; H. J. Croze; P. C. Lee (eds.). The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal. University of Chicago Press.
  6. ^ Estes, R. (1991). The behavior guide to African mammals: including hoofed mammals, carnivores, primates. University of California Press. pp. 263–66. ISBN 978-0-520-08085-0.
  7. ^ Fuchs, Evelyn; Beeck, Veronika C.; Baotic, Anton; Stoeger, Angela S. (2021-11-23). "Acoustic structure and information content of trumpets in female Asian elephants (Elephas maximus)". PLOS ONE. 16 (11): e0260284. Bibcode:2021PLoSO..1660284F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0260284. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 8610244. PMID 34813615.
  8. ^ "What Elephant Calls Mean: A User's Guide". National Geographic. 2014-05-02. Archived from the original on February 18, 2021. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  9. ^ "World Elephant Day: 25 wild animal facts". CBS News. 12 August 2015. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  10. ^ Kingdon, p. 63.
  11. ^ "Everything you ever wanted to know about Elephants". WildTrails. 2016-04-09. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  12. ^ a b Sukumar, R. (11 September 2003). The Living Elephants: Evolutionary Ecology, Behaviour, and Conservation. Oxford University Press, USA. ISBN 978-0-19-510778-4. OCLC 935260783.
  13. ^ Pardo, Michael A.; Fristrup, Kurt; Lolchuragi, David S.; Poole, Joyce H.; Granli, Petter; Moss, Cynthia; Douglas-Hamilton, Iain; Wittemyer, George (2024-06-10). "African elephants address one another with individually specific name-like calls". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 8 (7): 1353–1364. Bibcode:2024NatEE...8.1353P. doi:10.1038/s41559-024-02420-w. ISSN 2397-334X. PMID 38858512.
  14. ^ a b Herbest, C. T.; Stoeger, A.; Frey, R.; Lohscheller, J.; Titze, I. R.; Gumpenberger, M.; Fitch, W. T. (2012). "How Low Can You Go? Physical Production Mechanism of Elephant Infrasonic Vocalizations". Science. 337 (6094): 595–599. Bibcode:2012Sci...337..595H. doi:10.1126/science.1219712. PMID 22859490. S2CID 32792564.
  15. ^ Payne, K.B.; Langbauer, W.R.; Thomas, E.M. (1986). "Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 18 (4): 297–301. Bibcode:1986BEcoS..18..297P. doi:10.1007/BF00300007. S2CID 1480496.
  16. ^ Larom, D.; Garstang, M.; Payne, K.; Raspet, R.; Lindeque, M. (1997). "The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 200 (Pt 3): 421–31. doi:10.1242/jeb.200.3.421. PMID 9057305.
  17. ^ "Elephants Have Names for Each Other, Study Finds". Yale E360. Retrieved 2024-06-14.
  18. ^ a b Herbest, C. T.; Švec, J. G.; Lohscheller, J.; Frey, R.; Gumpenberger, M.; Stoeger, A.; Fitch, W. T. (2013). "Complex Vibratory Patterns in an Elephant Larynx". Journal of Experimental Biology. 216 (21): 4054–4064. doi:10.1242/jeb.091009. PMID 24133151.
  19. ^ O'Connell-Rodwell, E.O. (2007). "Keeping an "ear" to the ground: seismic communication in elephants". Physiology. 22 (4): 287–94. doi:10.1152/physiol.00008.2007. PMID 17699882.
  20. ^ O'Connell-Rodwell C. E.; Arnason, B.; Hart, L. A. (2000). "Seismic properties of Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) vocalizations and locomotion". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 108 (6): 3066–72. Bibcode:2000ASAJ..108.3066O. doi:10.1121/1.1323460. PMID 11144599.
  21. ^ O'Connell-Rodwell, C. E.; Wood, J. D.; Rodwell, T. C.; Puria, S.; Partan, S. R.; Keefe, R.; Shriver, D.; Arnason, B. T.; Hart, L. A. (1 February 2006). "Wild elephant (Loxodonta africana) breeding herds respond to artificially transmitted seismic stimuli" (PDF). Behavioural and Ecological Sociobiology. 59 (6): 842–50. Bibcode:2006BEcoS..59..842O. doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0136-2. S2CID 33221888. Archived (PDF) from the original on 3 December 2013.
  22. ^ a b c S., Albone, Eric (1984). Mammalian semiochemistry : the investigation of chemical signals between mammals. Wiley. ISBN 0-471-10253-9. OCLC 476223784.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ a b c d Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.A. (1998). "Chemical signals in the reproduction of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants". Animal Reproduction Science. 53 (1–4): 19–34. doi:10.1016/s0378-4320(98)00124-9. ISSN 0378-4320. PMID 9835364.
  24. ^ Von Dürckheim, Katharina (2021). Olfaction and scent discrimination in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) (Thesis). Stellenbosch University.
edit