Talk:Bateman's principle

Latest comment: 1 year ago by Agnerf in topic All illogical nonsense to me

Controversy?

edit

To call Bateman's principle a controversy, or to consider Bateman's fly work as the only evidence in support is pretty nutbar. I havn't read Judson's 'Dr. Tatania' book, but I gather she was a Bill Hamilton student, and would be really surprised to hear that she describes Bateman's principle as controversial. In any case, I'd like to see references to the papers by "Some modern evolutionary biologists"...

I've moved deleted the section from the article and move it below. The last paragraph about DNA fingerprinting showing true monogamy to be rare is totally besides the point of Bateman's principle.

Pete.Hurd 07:10, 31 December 2005 (UTC)Reply

Judson says in her book "Bateman's principle has a fundamental flaw: it's wrong." The chapter where she discusses it is posted as an online sample here.
Thanks for that link Lyrl, Judson means Bateman's principle to means that females have only the minimum amount of sex to insure fertilization, rather than definition used in the WP page: sex differences in initial parental investment producing sex differences in mating behaviour. Given her definition she's certainly right, I'm just not sold on her definition being what is usually meant by "Bateman's principle". I'll check a bunch of sources later on today and see what definition they use. Cheers, Pete.Hurd 19:28, 31 December 2005 (UTC)Reply
I've modified the proposed section below. Hopefully it's more accurate/relevent now.Lyrl 15:54, 31 December 2005 (UTC)Reply

She argues against the statments "a female's reproductive success is not increased by mating with more males" and "a male's reproductive success increases with each female he mates" (see modified section, below), both of which are quoted in the WP definition.

She also argues against the principle that "fertility is seldom likely to be limited by sperm production but rather by the number of inseminations or the number of females available to him" quoted in this article. This part isn't in the online sample, but she points to sea animals that do not mate, but rather emit sperm and eggs into the sea - each is equally unlikely to get fertilized because of the location issues. She also refers to flowers, where pollen delivery by pollinators is the limiting factor in reproduction. And then goes on to list a variety of animals where the male actually runs out of sperm - garter snakes, zebra finches, blue crabs, and rams (it apparently can take several days before they're capable of impregnanting a female again). And she then gives several examples of hermaphrodites where their mating behavoir indicates that sperm production is more expensive than egg production. She also supports this argument by saying females prefer to mate with virgin males (with presumably higher sperm counts)

This article also has a quote from Bateman "there is nearly always a combination of an undiscriminating eagerness in the males and a discriminating passivity in the females," where Judson spends an entire chapter of her book talking about female promiscuity, and sometimes males lacking 'undiscriminating eagerness.'Lyrl 23:46, 31 December 2005 (UTC)Reply

Yes, there are many well-known exceptions to Bateman's principle, and it would be very useful to document these in the article. I think the quote above ought to be included in the article, with some of the discussion you have below about the "nearly always" part. Williams (1966) pointed out the sex-role reversed counter examples, such as pipefish (seahorses) phalaropes, etc. There's quite a literature on sperm limitation in orthopterans that's relevant. The Judson piece seems like a good place to start, at least in classifying counter-examples, eg, 1) sex role reversal, 2) sperm limitation examples, 3) etc... Off hand I can't say who discussed such things pre-Williams, but it would be really good to discuss all these if-ands-and-buts in a historical context, which ought to lead to Trivers' 1972 reworking of the concept into his parental investment theory. Cheers, Pete.Hurd 16:50, 1 January 2006 (UTC)Reply

"Controversy" section removed from article

edit

At least one evolutionary biologist believes Bateman's principle is often not correct. Olivia Judson argues that Bateman's conclusions were limited by such things as short observation time in his experiments.

Observation of many species, from rabbits to slippery dicks (a type of fish) to fruit flies, has shown that females have more children if they have sex with more males. This is in contradiction to Bateman's theory that "a female's reproductive success is not increased by mating with more males."

She also points to research in which males will guard one female and mate only with her, attempting to prevent her from mating with any other males. Examples include stick insects and Idaho ground squirrels. These observations seem to challenge Bateman's theory that "a male's reproductive success increases with each female he mates."

Exceptions list - proposed to replace "controversy" section

edit
  • The statements "a female's reproductive success is not increased by mating with more males" and "a male's reproductive success increases with each female he mates" are (sometimes? frequently?) false.

Judson is arguing against these statments, and so only gives counter-examples, of course. One of Judson's counter-examples is the fruit fly Bateman studied, so I'm not sure the older references are reliable anymore. Any suggestions for recent sources that gave supporting evidence for these statements? Or old sources that are felt to still be reliable?

  • The assumption that females have a relatively larger investment in producing offspring can be false. In animals that spawn into the sea, for example, each sex's investment is approxamately equal. In animals with internal fertilization, many sperm must be produced for every egg - so even though it takes less energy to create one sperm than one egg, males of many species spend more energy making gametes than do females.
  • The statement that the sex that invests the most in producing offspring will become a limiting resource is not always true. In flowers, for example, the female part of the flower invests more energy into making seeds than the male part of the flower does. The reproduction of (most?) flowing plants, however, is limited by delivery of the male gamete - pollen - not by production of the female gamete.
  • Bateman's statement "there is nearly always a combination of an undiscriminating eagerness in the males and a discriminating passivity in the females" and his assumption that anisogamous species would be polygynous is false. [Many examples of female promiscuity]

That should be a start, at least. I'm not sure all the bullets fit under the category of 'exceptions', though; at least a couple of them seem to be arguing directly with Bateman's principle as it is defined in this article.Lyrl 22:56, 3 January 2006 (UTC)Reply

A Healthy Debate!

edit

Owing to numerous grammatical mistakes and a lack of clarity in the argument, it is difficult to divine exactly what the writer above takes exception to in the claim that there may be some problems with Bateman’s principle. Instead the author dismisses the notion that there is “controversy” over Bateman’s principle as “nutbar.” Yet even a cursory visit to the internet shows that there is considerable debate in the scientific community about Bateman’s propositions. One could look at Jonathan Knight’s article, “Sexual Stereotypes,” found in Nature (17 Jan 2002) or read through the range of papers delivered at a symposium entitled “Bateman’s Principles: Is it Time for Re-evaluation?” (University of Missouri, St. Louis) to understand that, in keeping with the scientific process, there is a vibrant and necessary debate on the various assertions and implications of Bateman’s principle. It is, we must remember, not a law. And in the normal course of scientific inquiry, we continue to test the hypotheses to see if they stand up to scrutiny now permitted by technological and scientific advances and the questioning of formerly unacknowledged social blinders.

It seems clear, at the very least, that some of the basic tenets of Bateman’s principle do not apply as widely or consistently in the natural world as was once believed. For reading on related topics, one should certainly enjoy the accessible and informed work, Dr. Tatiana’s Sex Advice to All Creation by Olivia Judson (evolutionary biologist, research fellow at Imperial College in London). But Judson is not alone. Joan Roughgarden, Professor of Biological Sciences at Stanford University, offers a wide-reaching analysis of sexual and gender diversity in Evolution’s Rainbow. Marlene Zuk, Professor of Biology at the University of California, Riverside, argues that gendered assumptions about human behavior has historically hindered our ability to accurately interpret animal behavior and cautions us against looking for models for humans in nature. These are just a few of the many works on the fascinating topic of evolution and the myriad sexual practices of the animal kingdom.

What there should be no controversy over is that Bateman’s principle is still in need of study, critique, and testing. The scientific community and the general public are benefited when there is an exchange of ideas and new information. Without this we would still be living in a flat world. I’m sure, Angus John Bateman, as a scientist, would expect no less.—Preceding unsigned comment added by Mmason2 (talkcontribs) 22:09, 2 January 2006

Like I said above, I think that it's uncontroversial that the sex that is a limiting factor will be a resource that the other competes over, while the rule that anisogamy leads to polygyny is not always true. The if ands and buts relating to the second have long been documented, and should be collected together by form of violation and discussed in the article. "Nutbar" wasn't constructive, my apologies. Whether "Bateman's principle" is used to mean a) "limiting sex is contested resource", or b) "anisogamy leads to polygyny" is something I'm still looking into. The most recent edition of Alcock's book (the only standard textbook I can find just now that mentions Bateman's principle) uses a). Other mentions that implied definition a, in my non-systematic search through my bookshelf, were Trivers' 1972, and Eric Charnov's "Sex Allocation". I didn't find any clear uses of the b form of the definition, but I seem to have loaned out most of the books I'd expect to find them in. Pete.Hurd 23:42, 2 January 2006 (UTC)Reply

I second Pete.Hurd in pretty much all respects. Consider the evidence in support of Bateman's principle and you will see it's pretty much a case of exceptions proving the rule. Three main lines of evidence off the top of my head: 1. Polygny is the norm across diecious species (indeed for 708/849 societies described by Flinn and Low 1986). In fact it's pretty much the rule in mammals, where female care is extensive. 2. In species/populations where polyandry occurs, it is associated with male parental care. 3. the intensity of sexual selection and polygyny are associated with one another (Alexander et al 1979). This article is pretty much on the fringe! I suggest "Parental investment, sexual selection and sex ratios" by Kokko and Jennions for anyone wanting to get a feel for the latest consensus. It's available from Hanna Kokko's website at http://www.helsinki.fi/~hmkokko/Publ/index.html

Flinn, M.V. & Low, B.S. (1986). Resource distribution, social competition, and mating patterns in human societies. In: Ecological aspects of social evolution, D. Rubenstein & R. Wrangham (Eds.), pp. 217-243. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.

Alexander, R.D., Hoogland, J.L., Howard, R.D., Noonan, K.M. and Sherman, P.W. (1979) Sexual dimorphism and breeding systems in pinnipeds, ungulates, primates and humans, in Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective, N.A. Chagnon and W. Irons, Scituate, M.A.: Duxbury Press, 402-435.

Ianrickard (talk) 14:24, 22 November 2008 (UTC)Reply

The Birkhead and Judson references arguing against the principle were published within the past ten years. My understanding is this is a case of recently discovered information changing the consensus within the scientific community; the 1970s and 1980s studies reflect the consensus at that time, not the consensus today. If there is significant contemporary support for Bateman's principle, this article should certainly be updated; otherwise, I think it presents the different viewpoints in an acceptable manner. LyrlTalk C 01:18, 23 November 2008 (UTC)Reply

Who is Bateman?

edit

For the general reader it would be beneficial if this article provided some background on who this Bateman character is(Angus John Bateman or A.J. Bateman.) Even a very brief "A.J. Bateman was a biologist who..." type sentence would be great. To throw around his last name as though everyone knows who he is is a disservice to the readers. Also some sort of pointer or reference to his 1948 paper that started it all would also be nice(I think it is Bateman, A. J. "Intrasexual Selection in Drosophila." Heredity, 2 1948, but I'm no expert here.)—Preceding unsigned comment added by 129.100.252.112 (talk) 16:14, 28 February 2006

"Examples of violations" section

edit

The first paragraph in this section is misleading and requires more accurate wording. The author states "... to fruit flies, has shown that females have more children if they have sex with more males."

In humans, for example, it is not the number of males the female has sex with that determines whether they will have more children. Rather, it is the number of successful instances of procreative potential intercourse with a male (by "procreative potential intercourse" I mean that (1) the male is fertile; (2) the male engages in vaginal intercourse with the female and (3) the male ejaculates in the female's vagina during intercourse). Thus, a woman is just as likely to get pregnant on a given day if she has vaginal intercourse with 5 different male partners or vaginal intercourse 5 times with a single partner.

The second paragraph is interesting and (perhaps) --Njals Saga 01:32, 23 September 2007 (UTC) relevant. However, I believe the author places too much weight on this being a "challenge" to Bateman's theory via the use of only two examples. If we substitute published studies for species, I believe that most scientists would want to see more than two contradictory or null studies before making such a claim. For the vast majority of the animal kingdom, there is little doubt that the more instances of male sex with new females will increase that male's probability of genetic offspring.Reply

I believe the main article would be improved by "tightening up" the wording in the second paragraph above. I prefer the phrase "procreative potential intercourse" but am open to other terms. The whole issue of number of partners versus number of times one male had intercourse is an important distinction for the overall evaluation of Bateman's ideas.--Njals Saga 01:26, 23 September 2007 (UTC)Reply

Males emit fewer and fewer sperm with each act of closely-spaced intercourse. Females are more likely to get pregnant if they have sex with multiple "fully charged" males than if they have sex with one male multiple times (receiving fewer and fewer sperm with each successive act).
The second paragraph gives only selected examples from Judson's book, and her book offers only a few such descriptions from the files she says she has that are "bursting" with such examples. I don't think this article is trying to prove or disprove the theory within the article, it's only reporting on what respected scientists such as Judson are claiming. The two examples are just that, examples of what Judson is basing her argument on, not the entire foundation of her claims.
If each female only has sex with one male, then the more females a male mates with the more likely he is to have genetic offspring. If females mate with multiple males (as occurs in the large majority of species), then sperm competition comes into play. By going off to mate with other females, the male leaves his original partner available to other males, who may provide sperm that outcompetes his original deposit. In many species, the cost of sperm competition is so high that males are more successful in fathering offspring if they stay around just one female and concentrate on preventing her from having sex with other males. LyrlTalk C 00:25, 24 September 2007 (UTC)Reply

Explanation of October 2009 edits

edit

With some consideration to comments that have been made here, I have taken the liberty of modifying the encyclopedia entry to make a few important changes. I felt that the first two paragraphs of the description were somewhat redundant and misleading. The statement “Animals are therefore fundamentally polygynous, as a result of being anisogamous” can be better explained as males being promiscuous and females being selective as a result of anisogamy. While the original statement is accurate, it doesn’t address the difference between male and female motivations for reproduction, which is key to Bateman's principle.

I felt that a description of Bateman’s experiment was important to provide background for understanding Bateman’s conclusions and resulting principle.

Regarding the “Exceptions and counter-examples” section, I believe it is important to address the recent work reexamining Bateman’s original study using modern techniques. The work points out errors in the original study cropping up mainly because of knowledge or technology gaps present in the 1940s.

It is important to note that Bateman’s study, though flawed, still reflects the mating strategy of many species, and should be maintained as a valid theory. However, as recent work and debate on this forum has shown, many species have alternative reproductive strategies that can be explained by a wide variety of hypotheses. By acknowledging these hypotheses, we can better understand the limitations of Bateman’s principle, and demonstrate that all species’ sexual motivations cannot be explained by a small number of reductionist theories. I provided some examples of these alternative hypotheses. Seanbeckettvt (talk) 01:32, 1 October 2009 (UTC)Reply

Article is biased and needs work

edit

Bateman's Principle refers largely to the greater reproductive variance of males compared to females of most species. However, the article barely touches on reproductive variance.

The species in which there are sex "role reversals" are actually the exceptions that prove the rule: in these species females have a faster reproductive rate and greater assurance of genetic parentage compared to males (e.g., seahorses).

As the article stands stands now, the presentation is quite biased. The reader may get the erroneous impression that Bateman's Principle is invalid. As a general principle, it isn't. Memills (talk) 00:02, 11 March 2013 (UTC)Reply

Indeed, the wording needs tightening up. The examples of reversed gender roles in jacanas and pipefish is described as "Exceptions and counter-examples", but could equally be described as variations on Bateman's principle, and as conformation that his basic principle, that the resource committed decides the gender roles holds true even in situations where we see a gender reversal. I have read both Judson and Roughgarden and enjoyed them immensely, but reading them as "Bateman was simply wrong" is doing them both a disservice. Their texts (Dr. Tatjana and Evolutions Rainbow) are for a general audience, and they are tweaked to be interesting and readable. Petter Bøckman (talk) 22:53, 25 March 2013 (UTC)Reply
Here's an article that might serve to put some of the critique in perspective: [Much Ado About Sex Difference in Reproductive Sucess. Petter Bøckman (talk) 13:10, 27 March 2013 (UTC)Reply
That is a very relevant post by a respected researcher in the field. It, and the references noted therein, should be integrated into the article. Don't have the free time myself at the moment, but a good exercise for an editor with a bit of time... Memills (talk) 00:58, 28 March 2013 (UTC)Reply

Deleting section from IP user

edit

Hi, I'm just going to give fair warning here that I'm deleting the section Bateman's_principle#Incompatibility_with_other_sex_difference_claims created by IP user 2.70.73.74: https://en.wiki.x.io/w/index.php?title=Bateman's_principle&type=revision&diff=870494001&oldid=868549659

The section is not clear and relies too much on opinion and primary research. Let me know if you want to discuss. — ʀoyoтϵ 22:22, 21 February 2019 (UTC)Reply

Bad writing

edit

This article has only one clear and presumably accurate and important statement: " his principle can be expressed in three simple statements. The first is that male reproductive success increases with the number of mates they attempt to copulate with, while female reproductive success does not. The second is that male reproductive success will show greater variance than female. The third is that sexual selection will have a greater effect on the sex with greater variance in reproductive success." This paragraph shows there are three different principles, and therefore "Bateman's Principle" is not a useful term for discussing evidence. Please start over, throw this monster away, and explain what independent evidence there is on each of these three principles. And yes of course there are exceptions. List them and stop quibbling about the fact that all three principles have some degree of validity. Burressd (talk) 22:28, 28 March 2020 (UTC)Reply

All illogical nonsense to me

edit

Seems to me, from the domestic animals I've observed copulating, there is no evidence of mate selection whatsoever. Sheep, cattle, dogs, cats. The female could not possibly look any less interested in, or concerned about what is occurring. She exhibits no regard whatever for any characteristic of her partner. — Preceding unsigned comment added by 122.151.210.84 (talk) 16:33, 3 June 2022 (UTC)Reply

Female domestic animals cannot be choosy when humans select a male for them. The males of most species will fight over females if there are multiple males. Agnerf (talk) 11:11, 19 July 2023 (UTC)Reply