Juan Pascual-Leone (born 1933 in Spain) is a developmental psychologist and founder of the neo-Piagetian approach to cognitive development. He introduced this term into the literature[1] and put forward[2] key predictions about developmental growth of mental attention and working memory.[3][4]

Juan Pascual-Leone
Born1933
Valencia, Spain
NationalitySpanish, Canadian
Known forneo-Piagetian approach to cognitive development; Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO)
Scientific career
FieldsCognitive Developmental Psychology and Developmental Neuropsychology
InstitutionsYork University
Thesis Cognitive development and cognitive style: A general psychological integration.  (1969)
Doctoral advisorJean Piaget
Other academic advisorsHerman Witkin
Websitehttps://psyc.info.yorku.ca/health-profiles/index.php?dept=&mid=2764

Pascual-Leone pioneered descriptions of developmental cognitive growth from an organismic perspective, i.e. "from within" the subjects' task processing.[5][4] He contrasts this "metasubjective" perspective with the external observer's perspective taken in much psychological research and theory. His modeling of processing involves mental or metasubjective task-analysis, which yields estimates of task complexity from the subject's perspective.[6][7][8] Using this method he clarified distinctions between learning (including the learning of executive functions), maturational-developmental processes, and working memory, studying their interrelationships from within the subject's processing.

The Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO), is his general causal model of cognitive development, framed in terms of organismic operators, schemes, and principles.

Education

edit

Pascual-Leone studied medicine at the University of Valencia and specialized in psychiatry and neurology in Santander, on the north coast of Spain, and in Paris. His background as a medical doctor and neuropsychiatrist, and his experience studying psychology with Jean Piaget, contributed to a sophisticated understanding of Piaget's theory. The TCO is an expansion and reformulation that integrates the ideas of, among others, his mentors Jean Piaget and Herman Witkin.

Under the Supervision of Piaget

edit

In 1963–69, Pascual-Leone studied psychology at the University of Geneva, Switzerland, where, in 1964, he obtained his M.A. (Licence) in Experimental Child Development, and in 1969, his Ph.D. in psychology. Here, he was under the direct supervision of Jean Piaget (1896-1980), at the peak of his fame as child psychologist and constructivist-development researcher, whom he refers to as "my intellectual father in Psychology".[9] As one of the later graduate students of Piaget, he obtained first-hand knowledge of Piaget's theory, collaborating in Piaget's book on mental image. He was one of the first of Piaget's students to explicitly highlight shortcomings of the master's theory.

Under the Supervision of Witkin

edit

Pascual-Leone also studied under the American psychologist Herman Witkin (1916-1979). Witkin—a student of Max Wertheimer, a founder of Gestalt Psychology—conducted research on individual differences (cognitive styles) in cognitive and perceptional psychology as well as personality development; his focus was on psycho-social processes and cognitive differentiation.[10] Witkin was an innovator who pioneered, from an organismic perspective, theories of cognitive styles, psychological differentiation, and learning styles.[11]

Pascual-Leone defended his doctoral thesis in psychology[12] in Geneva, with Piaget and Witkin as supervisors. In 1964–65, Pascual-Leone did research at Witkin's laboratory at the State University of New York, Downstate Medical Center. This final doctoral research was done under Witkin's sponsorship and supervision with the help of a postdoctoral fellowship from the Foundations Fund for Research in Psychiatry. Working with Witkin influenced Pascual-Leone's later TCO theory, which was more process-analytical and developmental but in line with Witkin's theory.[9]

Early research into cognitive development

edit

Pascual-Leone's now-classic cognitive developmental research in the 1960s led to his seminal paper in 1970,[2] one of the 500 most cited papers in the field of psychology. In this work he proposed a mathematical model of endogenous mental-attention and explained how it develops as a function of chronological age in normal children. His findings demonstrated for the first time that, when measured behaviorally, children's mental-attentional capacity increases after the age of three, by one symbol-processing unit every other year until it reaches seven units at 15–16 years. Seven units, according to Miller[13] and Pascual-Leone,[2][14] is the maximum-capacity of mental/executive attention in adults (although adults may habitually apply about 5 units of this capacity). Pascual-Leone's mental-attention model was the first to quantify the effective complexity of processing stages in human development. Many of his ideas challenged the scientific establishment at the time. Commenting on this work, Barrouillet and Gaillard wrote: "Neo-Piagetian theories have the potential to account for most of the learning difficulties and developmental disorders by cumulating the strength of the functionalist and the structuralist approaches."[15]

Quantifying levels of cognitive development

edit

Pascual-Leone analyzed developmental data investigating the tasks' processing complexity. He assessed complexity by modeling the number of essential operators, relations, or schemes that children must simultaneously hold in mind to produce performance. In 1963, he inferred with his analyses that there is a maximum mental demand that each child's age group can cope with – the characteristic mental (M-) power of each developmental stage-level.[2] Only when the growth of mental power in a child is equal to or larger than the task's mental demand, can the child reliably solve a task. Today this information is well recognized; but in the nineteen seventies and eighties the idea was controversial. Pascual-Leone was the first to claim, via his task analyses, that the true organismic stages were in fact the sub-stages of Piaget.[9] Recently Arsalidou, Pascual-Leone, Johnson, Morris, and Taylor have produced data suggesting that these levels of functioning may be expressed in adults by incrementation of brain activity in the executive (prefrontal lobes, etc.) network.[16]

Later work

edit

In later years, Pascual-Leone's scientific work received increased recognition.[17][18][19][20][21][22] His theory has been validated in his laboratory[23][8][24][25][26][27][28] and by independent researchers, who, often without explicit reference to Pascual-Leone, have supported his original predictions and subsequent results, either using his tasks or with methods that converge with his.[29][30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40]

The impact of Pascual-Leone's work can be seen in three scholarly controversies: (1) the debate over Trabasso's critique of Pascual-Leone's 1970 paper;[2][41][42][43][44] (2) the multi-author discussion of Kemps, De Rammelaere, and Desmet[45] comparing Pascual-Leone's model of mental attention with Baddeley's model of working memory;[46][47][48] and (3) the discussion of Demetriou, Spanoudis, and Shayer's study on speed of processing, working memory, and general intelligence.[49][5] A further indicator of the interest in Pascual-Leone's work is evidenced by various published interviews.[9][50][51][52][53]

With Janice M. Johnson, Pascual-Leone has written a book presenting his theory and data supporting it, as well as his method of mental task analysis.[54] He has published numerous articles and chapters (some of which can be found in ResearchGate). In 2006, he received an honorary doctorate from the University of Cyprus and in 2018 was made a Fellow of the Association for Psychological Science.

Cognitive styles

edit

Pascual-Leone was among the earlier developmental researchers to emphasize the role of cognitive styles (e.g., field-dependence-independence, adaptive flexibility) and individual differences in cognitive development;[55][56] and he was the first to quantify mental-attention capacity throughout development. His research on children's and adults' mental-attentional capacity and on cognitive styles (which affect the person's propensity to experience and cope with misleading/conflicting situations versus facilitating situations), as well as his metasubjective ("from within") analysis, has opened up new perspectives for understanding cognitive processes; and has helped to clarify causal-developmental relations between affect/motivation, cognition, individual differences, and complex/conceptual processes.

Pascual-Leone's work can be classified under four diverse categories: (1) Dialectical constructivist epistemology, the theory of constructive operators, a model for quantifying mental/executive attention, and process task analysis; (2) Educational domains: math, visuospatial, logic, language, science education, giftedness; (3) Individual differences, cognitive styles, developmental neuropsychology, and brain semantics; and (4) Mental health: psychotherapy,[57][58] meditation, and human change.

Regarding his innovative work on individual differences as sources of difficulty/conflict in Piagetian tasks, Case and Edelstein have written:

"The nature of this conflict, Pascual-Leone asserted, is the same as the conflict elicited by Witkin's classic embedded figures test […] As a consequence, the pathways by which a subject must arrive at the solution to the two sorts of different tasks (field misleading and field facilitating) is different. For this reason Pascual-Leone predicted that field-dependent subjects would show a different developmental profile from field-independent subjects in any battery in which both types of developmental task are administered […] Pascual-Leone was able to predict the pattern of individual differences across a remarkably broad variety of Piagetian and psychometric tasks[12] […] In the general theoretical system that he evolved, what was seen as universal about development was its inexorable move towards greater complexity, as a result of biennial increases in mental power (M power). What was seen as individual about development were the particular situations to which different children were sensitive, and the particular styles or strategies that they evolved for dealing with these situations. Field independence was just one of the styles Pascual-Leone studied."[59]

The Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO)

edit

Pascual-Leone proposed the Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO) – an organismic cognitive-developmental theory that is neuropsychologically interpretable in terms of brain processes.[60][16][61][54] He was the first to quantify cognitive-processing limitations in novel problem-solving situations and at different stages of development, and was the first to use a formal, explicit method of process-task analysis to estimate the mental-attention demands of tasks.[7]

This theory explains human psychological functioning as the product of the dynamic combination of schemes (the brain's "software" – information/action bearing processes, embodied by cell assemblies or networks) and "hidden hardware" operators of the brain. Hidden operators acting on schemes generate thoughts, actions, and learning. They are brain-resource mechanisms that regulate functioning of (and can change) schemes. Piaget left these resources unexplained and often referred to them as principles like "regulations", "accommodation", and "equilibration". Operators intervene in all neuropsychological processes that are emergent and not automatized or overlearned. They include M-operator (mental-attention capacity, which explains the innate-developmental basis of working memory); I-operator (inhibition mechanism causing attentional interruption); L-operator (i.e., logical- structural learning capability); C-learning (content-learning capability); F-operator (i.e., the neo-Gestaltist "field effects", or "minimum principle", or "S-R compatibility"); etc. Table 1 shows different operators of the TCO and some of their key brain regions.[62][54]

Pascual-Leone's model of mental attention includes activation and inhibition processes. The activation component (M-capacity) boosts information schemes necessary for task performance. M-operator capacity is measured in terms of the maximum number of mental schemes (not strongly activated by the input, learning, or by strong affects) that can be boosted into working memory (i.e., mental focus or attentional centration) at any one time. The method of M-measurement presents task items that vary systematically in demand for mental attention. In this way an individual's M-capacity can be measured, in terms of the amount of attentional demand they can handle.[7][54] By refining the method and accumulating research evidence, Pascual-Leone reached interval scales of measurement of this mental-attentional load. Pascual-Leone also highlighted brain regions that sub-serve cognitive processes linked to his theoretical constructs, offering a tentative chronological map of their evolution.[54][62][63]

M-capacity is the key maturational causal factor of working memory. Working memory refers to all the schemes in a person's repertoire that are sufficiently activated (irrespective of the cause) to co-determine the ongoing process of representation or performance. There are other causes of scheme activation (including affects/emotions, overlearning/automatization, field factors); for this reason the size of working memory is often larger than the size of M-centration.[64][14][62]

Table 1. Some of TCO's hidden operators listed in a plausible order of evolutionary emergence

edit
Operator Description Main Brain Region
A Set of affective processes that intervene in motivation and attentive arousal. Brain stem, hypothalamus, extended amygdala, limbic system
C Both the process of content learning and the schemes derived from associative content learning. Thalamus; Broadmann primary & secondary areas
F(SOP) The field operator, which acts as a binding mechanism in the brain and brings closure to mental representations in a neoGestaltist manner. It often functions intertwined with the principle of Schemes' Overdetermination of Performance (SOP) All areas
LC The process of automatized logical-structural learning derived from C-learning through over-practice and automatization. Right hemisphere (RH)
T Temporarily and effortlessly collates sequences of figurative schemes, thus facilitating the coordination that constitutes distal objects. Hippocampal complex.

Occipito-temporal cortex

S Effortlessly coordinates relations of coexistence among activated schemes, during operative activity (praxis). Thereby it facilitates emergence of spatial schemes or schemas. Hippocampal complex.

Occipito-parietal cortex

LA, LB Logical-structural learning primed by strong affects, or by preferences of the personal-social being – including his/her emotions. Limbic system, orbito & medial prefrontal, inferotemporal, medial parietal cortex
I The attentional interrupt, which corresponds to the power of central active inhibition of unwanted schemes activated in the situation. Prefrontal, RH-medial & dorsolateral cortex. Basal ganglia.Thalamus
M Mental attentional capacity of the individual. Prefrontal, lateral & dorsolateral cortex. Basal ganglia. Thalamus.
LM Logical-structural learning caused by the effortful use of mental attentional capacity Left hemisphere tertiary, polymodal areas
E Executive schemes in the person's repertoire, for the task at hand. Prefrontal, lateral, dorsolateral, etc. frontopolar areas

Current work

edit

Pascual-Leone is now professor emeritus and senior scholar at York University, Toronto, where he continues to have an active laboratory. He has completed with Dr. Janice Johnson a longitudinal study on development of mental attentional capacity. With Dr. Marie Arsalidou he continues research on mental-attention measurement that is applicable across content domains (Color-Matching Task, Letter-Matching Task, Number Matching Task, etc.). These tasks are being compared with established mental-capacity tasks (such as Figural Intersections Task (FIT)) and are related to language (e.g., bilingualism) and other domains. They are also investigating brain correlates of the tasks in children and adults by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In a different direction, Pascual-Leone's laboratory is studying how to energize a person's functional mental attention (not his/her maturational capacity or reserve) by using meditation methods. His lab has previously shown that regular practice of Tai Chi increases the functional level of mental attention.[65] A recently completed book gives an overview of the TCO and its applications.[54]

References

edit
  1. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., & Smith, J. (1969). "The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: A new experimental paradigm and a neo-Piagetian model". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 328-355.
  2. ^ a b c d e Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). "A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages." Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345.
  3. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., (2011). "Piaget as a pioneer of dialectical constructivism: Seeking dynamic processes for human science." In E. Marti & C. Rodriguez (Eds.), After Piaget. (pp. 15-41) Edison, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
  4. ^ a b Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2017). "Organismic causal models "from within" clarify developmental change and stages". In N. Budwig, E. Turiel, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), New perspectives on human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  5. ^ a b Pascual-Leone, J. (2013). "Can we model organismic causes of working memory, efficiency and fluid intelligence? A meta-subjective perspective". Intelligence, 41, 738-743.
  6. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., Romero Escobar, E.M., & Johnson, J. (2012). Logic: Development of logical operations. In W. Hirstein (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (2nd ed.. pp. 538-549). New York, NY: Elsevier.
  7. ^ a b c Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2011). "A developmental theory of mental attention: Its applications to measurement and task analysis". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian and cognitive approaches (pp. 13-46). New York: Psychology Press.
  8. ^ a b Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2010). "Misleading cues improve developmental assessment of attentional capacity: The colour matching task". Cognitive Development, 25, 262-277.
  9. ^ a b c d Cardellini, L., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2004). "On cognitive mentors, cognitive development, education, and constructivism: An interview with Juan Pascual-Leone". Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 4, 199-219.
  10. ^ Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R.B., Faterson, H. G., Goodenough, D.R., & Karp, S.A. (1962). Psychological differentiation: Studies of development. New York, NY: Wiley.
  11. ^ Witkin, H. & Goodenough, D. R. (1981) Cognitive style: essence and origin. New York: International Universities Press (Psychological Issues # 51).
  12. ^ a b Pascual-Leone, J. (1969) Cognitive development and cognitive style: A general psychological integration. Ph.D. Thesis presented at the Faculty of Psychology and Science of Education. University of Geneva, Thesis No. 44, Geneva, Switzerland.
  13. ^ Miller, G. A. (1956). "The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information". Psychological Review. 63 (2): 81–97. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.308.8071. doi:10.1037/h0043158. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4646-B. PMID 13310704. S2CID 15654531.
  14. ^ a b Pascual-Leone, J. (1984). "Attention, dialectic, and mental effort: Towards an organismic theory of life stages". In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & G. Armon (Eds.) Beyond formal operations: Late adolescence and adult cognitive development (pp. 182-215). NY: Praeger.
  15. ^ Barrouillet, P., & Gaillard, V. (2011). "Advances and issues: Some thoughts about controversial questions". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian and cognitive approaches (pp. 263-271). New York: Psychology Press. p. 270
  16. ^ a b Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., Morris, D., & Taylor, M. J. (2013). "A balancing act of the brain: Activations and deactivations driven by cognitive load". Brain and Behavior. doi:10.1002/brb3.128.
  17. ^ Bideau, J., Houde, O., & Pedinielli, J-L. (1995). L'homme en developpement. Paris: PUF.
  18. ^ Lautrey, J. (Ed.) (2006). Psychologie du développement et psychologie différentielle (pp. 76-102). Paris: PUF.
  19. ^ Morra, S., Gobbo, C., Marini, Z., & Sheese, R. (2008). Cognitive development: Neo-Piagetian perspectives. New York, NY: Erlbaum.
  20. ^ de Ribaupierre, A, (2016) "Memoire de travail, developpement cognitif et performances scolaires". In P-A. Doudin & E. Tardif (Eds.) Neurosciences et cognition: Perspectives pour les sciences de l'education. (pp. 165-188). Louvain-la-Neuve – Belgique: De Boeck, superieur.
  21. ^ Troadec, B., & Martinot, C. (2003). Le développement cognitif: Theories actuelles de la pensee en contextes. Paris: Belin.
  22. ^ Vuyk, R. (1981) Overview and critique of Piaget's genetic epistemology 1965-1980 (Vol. II). London, U.K: Academic Press.
  23. ^ Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). "Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 286-305.
  24. ^ Balioussis, C., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2012) "Fluency and complexity in children's writing: The role of mental attention and executive function". Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata / Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 33-45.
  25. ^ Burtis, P. J. (1982). "Capacity increase and chunking in the development of short-term memory". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 387-413.
  26. ^ Howard, S. J., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2013). "Measurement of mental attention: Assessing a cognitive component underlying performance on standardized intelligence tests". Psychological Assessment and Test Modeling, 55(3), 250-272.
  27. ^ Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). "Processing limitation in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function". Child Development, 77, 1822-1841.
  28. ^ Johnson, J., Fabian, V., & Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). "Quantitative hardware-stages that constrain language development". Human Development, 32, 245-271.
  29. ^ Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1979). "Pascual-Leone's M construct as a link between cognitive-developmental and psychometric concepts of intelligence". Intelligence, 3, 41-63.
  30. ^ Onwumere, O., & Reid, N. (2014). "Field dependency and performance in mathematics". European Journal of Educational Research, 3, 43-57.
  31. ^ Cowan, N. (2014). "Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education". Educational Psychology Review, 26, 197-223.
  32. ^ Cowan, N. (2016). "Working memory maturation: Can we get at the essence of cognitive growth?" Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 239-264.
  33. ^ Case, R. (1998). "The development of conceptual structures". In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.; pp. 745-800). New York, NY: Wiley.
  34. ^ Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). "The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1-2, Serial No. 246).
  35. ^ Niaz, M. (2006). "Can the study of thermochemistry facilitate students' differentiation between heat energy and temperature?" Journal of Science Education and Technology, 15, 269-276.
  36. ^ Mella, N., Fagot, D. Lecerf, T. & de Ribaupierre, A. (2014) "Working memory and intraindividual variability in processing speed: A lifespan developmental and individual-differences study". Memory & Cognition, vol. 42, 8, November. doi:10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1.
  37. ^ Morra, S. Camba, R., Guglielmo Calvini, G & Bracco, F. (2013) "Italians do it better? M-capacity measurement and cross-linguistic differences in the Direction Following Task (DFT)". Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata / Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, xiii, 1/2013.
  38. ^ Morra, S & Muscella, L. (2016) "A reappraisal of the Bose-Einstein model of the CSVI task : New experimental evidence, replications, and implications". EWOMS-8 - Liège, September 1, 2016.
  39. ^ de Ribaupierre, A., Fagot, D., & Lecerf, T. (2011). "Working memory capacity and its role in cognitive development". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory (pp. 105-133). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  40. ^ Tsaparlis, G. & Angelopoulus, V. (2000). "A model of problem-solving: Its operation, validity, and its usefulness in the case of organic-synthesis problems". Journal of Science Education, 84,131–153.
  41. ^ Pascual-Leone, J. (1978). "Compounds, confounds and models in developmental information processing: A reply to Trabasso and Foellinger". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 18-40.
  42. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., & Sparkman, E. (1980). "The dialectics of empiricism and rationalism: A last methodological reply to Trabasso". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 88-101.
  43. ^ Trabasso, T. (1978). "On the estimation of parameters and the evaluation of a mathematical model: A reply to Pascual-Leone". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 41-45.
  44. ^ Trabasso, T., & Foellinger, D. B. (1978). "Information processing capacity in children: A test of Pascual-Leone's model". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 1-17.
  45. ^ Kemps, E., De Rammelaere, S., & Desmet, T. (2000). "The development of working memory: Exploring the complementarity of two models". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 89-109.
  46. ^ Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2000). "Development of working memory: Should the Pascual-Leone and the Baddeley and Hitch models be merged?" Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 128-137.
  47. ^ Pascual-Leone, J. (2000). "Reflections on working memory: Are the two models complementary?" Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 138-154.
  48. ^ de Ribaupierre, A., & Bailleux, C. (2000). "The development of working memory: Further note on the comparability of two models of working memory". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 110-127.
  49. ^ Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., & Shayer, M. (2013). "Developing intelligence: Is a comprehensive theory possible?" Intelligence, 41, 730-731.
  50. ^ Corral, A. & del Valle, Ch. & Pascual-Leone, J. (1993). "Entrevista con Pascual-Leone: Sobre inteligencia artificial, creatividad, inteligencia verdadera, voluntad, aprendizaje y desarrollo". Tarbiya: Revista del Instituto de Ciencias de la Educación. Universidad Antonoma de Madrid, 515-27.
  51. ^ Delval, J., with Pascual-Leone, J. (1977). "Entrevista con Juan Pascual-Leone". Cuadernos de Psicologia (Madrid, Editorial Fundamentos), 8-9, 26-38.
  52. ^ Society for Research in Child Development oral history project
  53. ^ Canadian Psychological Association oral history project
  54. ^ a b c d e f Pascual-Leone J. & Johnson, J. M. (2021). The working mind: Meaning and mental attention in human development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  55. ^ Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). "An organismic process model of Witkin's field-dependence-independence". In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36-70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  56. ^ Shafrir, U., & Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). "Postfailure reflectivity/impulsivity and spontaneous attention to errors". Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 378-387.
  57. ^ Pascual-Leone, J. (1991). "Emotions, development and psychotherapy: A dialectical constructivist perspective". In J. Safran & L. Greenberg (Eds.), Emotion, psychotherapy and change (pp. 302-335). New York: Guilford.
  58. ^ Greenberg, L., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2001). "A dialectical constructivist view of the creation of personal meaning". Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 14, 165-186.
  59. ^ Case, R., & Edelstein, W. (Eds.). (1993). The new structuralism in cognitive development: Theory and research on individual pathways. Basel, Switzerland: Karger. p. 5-6
  60. ^ Pascual-Leone, J. (1995). "Learning and development as dialectical factors in cognitive growth". Human Development, 38, 338-348.
  61. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., Pascual-Leone, A., & Arsalidou, M. (2015). "Neuropsychology still needs to model organismic processes 'from within'". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e83.
  62. ^ a b c Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2005). "A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence". In O. Wilhelm & R. Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 177-201). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  63. ^ Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., & Agostino, A. (2010). "Mental attention, multiplicative structures, and the causal problem of cognitive development". In M. Ferrari & L. Vuletic (Eds.), Developmental interplay between mind, brain and education: Essays in honor of Robbie Case (pp. 49-82). New York, NY: Springer.
  64. ^ Johnson, J., Im-Bolter, N., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2003). "Development of mental attention in gifted and mainstream children: The role of mental capacity, inhibition, and speed of processing". Child Development, 74, 1594-1614.
  65. ^ Kim, T., Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J. & Tamim, H. (2016). "The mental-attention Tai Chi effect with older adults". BMC Psychology, 4:29.